To summarize this set of properties, the red–green and yellow–blue channels are inferior to
the luminance channel in almost every respect. The general implications for data display are clear.
Purely chromatic differences should never be used for displaying object shape, object motion, or
detailed information such as text. From this perspective, color would seem almost irrelevant and
certainly a secondary method for information display. Nevertheless, when it comes to coding
information, using color to display data categories is usually the best choice. To see why, we need
to look beyond the basic processes that we have been considering thus far.
Color Appearance
The value of color (as opposed to luminance) processing, it would appear, is not in helping us
to understand the shape and layout of objects in the environment. Color does not help the hunter
aim an arrow accurately. Color does not help us see shape from shading and thereby plan a hike
through a valley, although it does help us distinguish vegetation types. Color does not help use
stereoscopic depth when we reach out to grasp a tool. But color is useful to the gatherer. Food,
in the forest or on the savannah, is often distinct because of its color. This is especially true of
fruits and berries. Color creates a kind of visual attribute of objects: this is a red berry. That is
a yellow door. Color names are used as adjectives because colors are perceived as attributes of
objects. This suggests a most important role for color in visualization—namely, the coding of
information. Visual objects can represent complex data entities, and colors can naturally code
attributes of those objects.
Color is normally a surface attribute of an object. The XYZ tristimulus values of a patch of
light physically define a color, but they do not tell us how it will look. Depending on the sur-
rounding colors in the environment and a whole host of spatial and temporal factors, the same
physical color can look very different. If it is desirable that color appearance be preserved, it is
important to pay close attention to surrounding conditions. In a monitor-based display, a large
patch of standardized reference white will help ensure that color appearance is preserved. When
colors are reproduced on paper, viewing them under a standard lamp will help preserve their
appearance. In the paint and fabric industries, where color appearance is critical, standard
viewing booths are used. These booths contain standard illumination systems that can be set to
approximate daylight or a standard indoor illuminant, such as a typical tungsten light bulb or
halogen lamp.
The mechanisms of surface lightness constancy, discussed at some length in Chapter 3, gen-
eralize to trichromatic color perception. Both chromatic adaptation and chromatic contrast occur
and play a role in color constancy. Differential adaptation in the cone receptors helps us to
discount the color of the illumination in the environment. When there is colored illumination,
different classes of cone receptors undergo independent changes in sensitivity. Thus, when the
illumination contains a lot of blue light, the short-wavelength cones become relatively less sen-
sitive than the others. The effect of this is to shift the neutral point at which the three receptor
types are in equilibrium, such that more blue light must be reflected from a surface for it to seem
116 INFORMATION VISUALIZATION: PERCEPTION FOR DESIGN
ARE4 1/20/04 5:00 PM Page 116