
of combining diff erent features that will come to be identifi ed as parts of
the same contour or region is called binding . ere is no such thing as an
object embedded in an image; there are just patterns of light, shade, color,
and motion. Objects and patterns must be discovered, and binding is
essential because it is what makes disconnected pieces of information into
connected pieces of information.
To explain how the responses of individual feature-detecting neurons
may become bound into a pattern representing the continuous edge of an
object, let us indulge in a little anthropomorphism and imagine ourselves
to be a single neuron in the primary visual cortex. As a neuron, we are far
more complex than a transistor in a computer—our state is infl uenced by
incoming signals from thousands of other neurons; some excite us, some
inhibit us. When we get overexcited we fi re , shooting a convulsive burst
of electrical energy along our axon. Our outgoing axon branches out and
ultimately infl uences the fi ring of thousands of other neurons. Supposing
we are the kind of neuron in the primary visual cortex that responds to
oriented edge information, we get most excited when part of an edge or a
contour falls over a particular part of the retina to which we are sensitive.
is exciting information may come from almost anything, part of the sil-
houette of a nose or just a bit of oriented texture.
Inputs
axon
Outputs
Thousands of inputs are received on the dendrites. These signals are combined in the cell body.
The neuron fires an electrical spike of energy along the axon, which branches out at the end
to influence other neurons, some positively, some negatively. Neurons fire all the time, and
information is carried by both increases and decreases in the rate.
Now comes the interesting bit. In addition to the information from
the retina, we also receive input from neighboring neurons and we set
up a kind of mutual admiration society with some of them, egging them
on as they egg us on. To form part of our edge-detecting club, they must
respond to part of the visual world that is in-line with our preferred axis,
and they must be attuned to roughly the same orientation. When these
conditions are met, we start to pulse in unison sending out pulses of elec-
trical energy together.
It is never acceptable to simply say
that a higher level mechanism looks
at the low-level information. That is
called the homunculus fallacy. It is like
saying that there is an inner person (a
homunculus) that sees the information
on the retina or in the primary visual
cortex. A proper explanation is that
a set of mechanisms and processes
produce intelligent actions as an
outcome of interactions between
processes operating in sub-systems of
the brain.
The Binding Problem: Features to Contours 47
CH03-P370896.indd 47CH03-P370896.indd 47 1/24/2008 4:32:06 PM1/24/2008 4:32:06 PM